inner-banner-bg

Journal of Clinical Pediatrics and Child Care Research(JCPCCR)

ISSN: 2832-2584 | DOI: 10.33140/JCPCCR

Impact Factor: 1.10

Review Article - (2024) Volume 5, Issue 2

The Influence of Using Baby Walkers on the Infant Motor Development: Systematic Review

Samah Saud Alharbi 1 *, Weaam Alammar 2 and Halah Abuaish 2
 
1King Faisal Specialist Hospital and research Center, Physical Rehabilitation Department, Saudi Arabia
2Senior Pediatric Physical Therapist at King Faisal Specialist Hospital, Saudi Arabia
 
*Corresponding Author: Samah Saud Alharbi, King Faisal Specialist Hospital and research Center, Physical Rehabilitation Department, Saudi Arabia

Received Date: Aug 16, 2024 / Accepted Date: Sep 06, 2024 / Published Date: Sep 10, 2024

Copyright: �©2024 Samah Saud Alharbi, et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited.

Citation: Alharbi, S. S., Alammar, W., Abuaish, H. (2024). The Influence of Using Baby Walkers on the Infant Motor Development: Systematic Review. J Cli Ped Chi Res, 5(2), 01-09.

Abstract

Background: Research findings suggest that a significant percentage of families worldwide, ranging from 42% to 90%, utilize baby walkers for their infants. However, there is evidence indicating that baby walkers can delay the natural development of motor skills and may pose a risk of infants’ health. Report of injuries related to baby walker range from 7 % to 50 % depending on the source. Consequently, additional research is warranted to investigate the influence of using baby walker on healthy infant motor development.

Purpose: Review to explore the effectiveness of baby walker use on Infants motor development.

Methods: Seven out of eleven studies were observational cross-sectional studies, two studies were described as clinical trial, and other study was longitudinal study, and the last one semi-prospective case-control study, published between 2002-2023. Data source was collected from, Google Scholar, Cochrane library, ProQuest, articles in Scopus, and PEDro databases.

Data Extraction: any articles focused on Baby walker usage without considering the influence of infant motor development. Data Synthesis: The study focuses on the influence of using baby walker on healthy infant motor development.

Limitation of the Study: The review study is limited to 11 studies, stemming from inconsistencies in outcome and definitions across the articles reviewed, leading to the need to consolidate results for analysis

Conclusion: The literature review showed that there is a significant deficiency in evidence regarding the potential influence of baby walkers’ child motor development.

Keywords

Healthy Infant, Baby Walker, Motor Development

Introduction

The prioritization of gross motor development in early childhood is achieved through the provision of proper stimulation. However, a significant obstacle is limited knowledge of parents in selecting appropriate methods of stimulation. Traditionally, baby walkers have been a popular choice for parents looking to encourage early walking in infant between 4 to 12 months of age, dating back to the 1660s [1]. However, according to previous research conducted in Indonesia indicate that the average age at which infants begin to walk is 9-15 months [2]. This disparity highlights the variability in children's developmental timelines and underscores the signif-
icance of recognizing individual differences in the developmental progression. Generally, infants typically start walking between 9 to 12 months of age. With the most babies achieving independent walking by 15 months.

According to Janusz et al, parents have confidence in the ability of baby walkers to assist infants in maintaining contact with the ground and facilitating movement while they develop their walk- ing skills [3]. Additionally, baby walkers are seen as a useful tool for strengthening the lower body muscles in infants as they begin to walk. However, previous research has indicated that prolonged use of baby walkers in infants can determinedly influence gross motor development and lead to injuries resulting from falls [4,5]. There is concern regarding the efficacy of baby walker based on age can effectively enhance motor skills in infants. An added bene- fit of using a baby walker is its ability to aid in the learning to walk process; assisting infants who are unable to navigate and explore their surrounding while in a standing position [6].

Despite it is long history of usage, there has been ongoing debate surrounding the safety and effectiveness of baby walkers in recent years. While baby walkers continue to be widely utilized global- ly statistics indicate that approximately 50%-77% of parents of infants use baby walker [5,7]. Parents’ attitudes to use BW vary based on their cultural beliefs and lifestyles.

Some pediatric associations do not recommend their use due to safety concerns [8,9]. In fact, safety standards for baby walkers have been revised in the USA to address potential hazards such as fall and accidents [8,9]. Despite the warring, many families contin- ue to use baby walkers as a way to keep their child occupied and to potentially accelerate their development [5]. However, studies examining the effects of baby walker use on child development have yielded conflicting results. Some studies have reported ad- verse effects such as delays in motor skills, while others reporting no such correlations [10,11].

As Badihian et al, highlighted the limited research available on the impact of baby walker use on child development [4]. While few studies have assessed this relationship, there is insufficient ev- idence to definitely conclude whether baby walker are beneficial to child’s development. The Current data in the literature still are not conclusive and need more research is needed to fully understand the relationship [4,12]. The potential implications of developmen- tal delay associated with the use of baby walkers can be approached from two perspectives [4]. Firstly, these walkers may encourage premature locomotion in infant, potentially disrupting the natural developmental process that infants need to undergo [13]. Second- ly, they may hinder the visual experience of infants moving limbs due to their design, which is believed to play a crucial role in the development pf motor skills. Because of these concerns, including injuries related to the use of walkers, baby walker sales have been prohibited in Canada since 1989, and the American Academy of pediatrics does not recommend their use [8,12]. This paper does a systematic review to explore the effectiveness of baby walker use on Infants motor development.

Methodology

Methods
Systematic review of the literature was performed through elec- tronic search from August 2023. By identifying the studies from Google Scholar, Cochrane library, ProQuest, Scopus, and PEDro databases form the August 2023-August 2024 period. All the arti- cles, which filled the inclusion criteria related to healthy children who used baby walker during the first 18 months of age, were in-cluded without considering the year of publication.

These search engines include Specific search words were used to filter out unnecessary and irrelevant articles. The search words used were topic-related and were targeted to get the best results from the search engines such as (baby walker AND motor development), (baby walker AND infant gait), (baby walker AND crawling), (de- velopment OR walk), (baby walker OR runner), (baby walker Or infant walker), (pediatric OR Walking aid).

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria
The inclusion of articles in this review was based on the following:
•    The chosen articles in English language focus on clinical trials, randomized control trials, pilot studies, feasibility studies, and systematic reviews that critically evaluated the influence of baby walker on the child development. The interventions recommended for these studies involve physical therapy utilizing baby walkers as a tool for enhancing infants’ motor development. Studies that had evaluated the influence of baby walker on health child devel- opment from 4-18 months of age, since babies can use walkers as young as 4 months while the majority of infants walk independent- ly by 18 months. The desired outcome of these studies is to analyze the influence of baby walker usage on infants’ motor development.
•    Studies focusing on children with physical or developmental im- pairment prior to commencing walker use were excluded.

 Quality of Methodological Reporting
The methodological reporting quality of study was analyzed us- ing the PEDRo scale. All studies met the initial criteria related to participant selection and intervention with the baby walkers, as well as having comparable groups before intervention (criteria #4), only two studies adhered to the criteria for (blinding of subjects), Criteria #5. While, none of the studies met the criteria for blinding of the therapists, Criteria #6. Most of the studies did not meet the criteria for one key outcome for at least 90 % of the subjects, Criteria #8. Intention to treat analysis Criteria #9, and statistical comparisons, Criteria #10. Various strategies were implemented to minimize bias, such as conducting a comprehensive search for published evidence in multiple databases.

 Study Selection

Two investigators selected studies for eligibility based on the pre- ferred reporting items for systematic reviews and Meta-analysis (PRISMA) statement [14]. They conducted a thorough examina- tion of randomized controlled trails, pilot studies, and clinical trials that investigated the influence of baby walkers on infants’ motor development. The primary researcher (SA) independently reviewed abstracts to identify articles related to the use of baby walkers and functional motor development. A recursive search of the references from relevant articles was also conducted. Articles were evaluated by second (WA) and third reviewers (HA) to en- sure they met the criteria for inclusion. The extracted data were entered into sheet to be compared as reported in this paper.

Results

A total of 141,000 articles were identified in Google Scholar, 5 in Cochrane library, 168,041 papers in ProQuest, 21 articles in Sco- pus, and 1 review in PEDro databases. The process of reviewing the titles and abstracts, followed by a thorough assessment of the full manuscripts of pertinent articles, resulted in the selection of 10 articles that adhere to our criteria, encompassing one clinical trial, eight observational studies, one longitudinal study, and one semi-prospective case-control study. Figure displays the flowchart depicting the process of identification, screening and selection.
 

<img src="https://www.opastpublishers.com/scholarly-images/8039-68242c66f2a51-the-influence-of-using-baby-walkers-on-the-infant-motor-deve.png" style="width:400px;height:400px;">

Author/ Year

Study De- sign

Sample (N)

Duration of follow up

Age at walker usage

Frequency of walker use

Outcome measure- ment/ Inter- vention

Finding

Limitations

Janusz et al

Cross-Sec-

969

-

8.7 months

- Median

- The mean

- The onset of

- Recall

[3]

tional

 

 

(Mean age)

number of

age of onset

walking was earli-

bias, since

 

Survey

 

 

 

days of baby

of walking

er in children using

the partic-

 

 

 

 

 

walker use

 

baby walker less

ipant were

 

 

 

 

 

per week 5

 

than 3 months

not able to

 

 

 

 

 

days

 

- Children who

recall details

 

 

 

 

 

- The mean

 

used a baby walker

of their

 

 

 

 

 

length of use

 

were 3 times more

children’s

 

 

 

 

 

per day was

 

likely to not have

walker use

 

 

 

 

 

1.9 hours

 

crawled for mobil-

since the

 

 

 

 

 

per day.

 

ity than those who

question-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

did not use a baby

naire was

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

walker.

administered

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

- Children demon-

with parent

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

strated gait disor-

whose chil-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ders (e.g. toe walk-

dren were

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ing and flat foot) in

around 5

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

both groups.

years of age.

Bezgin et al

Case-Con-

48

18 months

10.55

2.5 months

- The mean

- The use of baby

- Small

[15]

trol Study

 

 

months

(Median)

age of ac-

walkers might

sample size

 

 

 

 

(Mean age)

 

quisition of

delays the motor

of the study

 

 

 

 

 

 

motor skills

development of in-

partici-

 

 

 

 

 

 

including

fants and this may

pants and

 

 

 

 

 

 

rolling,

be due to impaired

the motor

 

 

 

 

 

 

crawling,

trunk control.

development

 

 

 

 

 

 

moving on

 

of children

 

 

 

 

 

 

hands and

 

cannot be

 

 

 

 

 

 

feet, sitting

 

followed

 

 

 

 

 

 

without and

 

after the

 

 

 

 

 

 

with help,

 

study.

 

 

 

 

 

 

standing

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

and walking

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

dependent-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ly (using

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

AIMS)

 

 

Yaghini et al

Cross-Sec-

214

At 12

1.46 months

Not reported

- Measure

- No significant

- Did not

[16]

tional Study

 

months

(Mean age)

 

gross motor

difference was

determine

 

 

 

then at 18

 

 

development

found between

baby walker

 

 

 

months

 

 

between

the two groups in

usage fre-

 

 

 

 

 

 

users and

the area of gross

quency

 

 

 

 

 

 

non-users

movements at the

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

(using ASQ)

age of 12 and 18

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

months

 

METE et al

Semi-Pro-

336

From birth

Not reported

30 minutes

- Assess lan-

- There was no

- Did not

[17]

spective

 

up to the age

 

or more

guage-cog-

developmental

determine

 

Case-Con-

 

of ten years

 

a day for

nitive, motor

problem in any of

the ages of

 

trol

 

for

 

at least 1

and mental

the children,

onset using

 

Study

 

well-child

 

month

develop-

- There was a

the baby

 

 

 

controls

 

 

ment. Sitting

statistically signif-

walker

 

 

 

 

 

 

without

icant

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

support and

difference in terms

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

independent

of an atypical gait

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

walking

pattern between

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

(Using

the

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ADSI)

two groups espe-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

cially toe-walking

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

were more fre-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

quent in children

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

who used BW.

 

Sharov et al

Pseudo-Ret-

268

Not reported

Not reported

18.5 months

The mean

- No difference be-

- Various

[18]

rospective

 

 

 

(Average

age of

tween two groups

frequencies

 

Cohort

 

 

 

frequency)

achieving

regarding stand-

of using

 

Study

 

 

 

 

standing

ing with support

baby walker

 

 

 

 

 

 

with support

and moving with

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

moving

support

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

around and

- A delay in

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

walking

walking alone

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

alone.

among children

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

who were put into

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

baby-walkers it

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

amounts to 10-13

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

days depending

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

on frequency and

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

time spent in ba-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

by-walkers a day

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

- Significant cor-

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

relation between

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

using baby walker

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

and tiptoe walking

 

Shafeek and

Clinical trial

87

Until ac-

Before 7

At least 3

- Identify the

- Delay the acqui-

- No infor-

El-Negmy

 

 

quisition of

months

hours daily

gross motor

sition of indepen-

mation on

[19]

 

 

independent

(Mean)

 

development

dent walking and

sample size

 

 

 

gait

 

 

for children

disturb the normal

calculation

 

 

 

 

 

 

(using ASQ-

gait pattern in

- Failure to

 

 

 

 

 

 

3)

normal children

define study

 

 

 

 

 

 

- The mean

 

population

 

 

 

 

 

 

age of gait

 

clearly

 

 

 

 

 

 

acquisitions

 

 

Alessa et al

Cross-Sec-

235

Not reported

Not reported

< 1 hour

- Children

- The baby walker

- Failure to

[20]

tional Study

 

 

 

daily (80

skipped sit-

enhance children

randomize

 

 

 

 

 

patients)

ting without

motor development

study group

 

 

 

 

 

1-2 hours

support

and skip major

- Not re-

 

 

 

 

 

daily (102

- Skipped

gross motor skills

ported ages

 

 

 

 

 

patients)

crawling

 

at onset of

 

 

 

 

 

2-4 hours

Skipped

 

using baby

 

 

 

 

 

daily (42

pulling to

 

walker

 

 

 

 

 

patients)

stand

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

> 4 hours

- Skipped

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

daily (11

cursing

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

patients)

 

 

 

Shiva et al

Cross-Sec-

414

Not reported

The age for

Not reported

The mean

No significant

Did not

[21]

tional Study

 

 

the onset of

 

age of gait

difference in the

determine

 

 

 

 

use of walk-

 

acquisitions

mean age of onset

baby walker

 

 

 

 

ers was 3–9

 

 

of independent

usage fre-

 

 

 

 

(mean age

 

 

walking was ob-

quency

 

 

 

 

5.59 ± 1.47)

 

 

served between the

 

 

 

 

 

months

 

 

two groups

 

Talebian et

Longitudinal

300

2 years

3-15 months

Not reported

The mean

- Baby walkers

- Incomplete

al [22]

Study

 

 

(no mean

 

age of ac-

delays the acquisi-

reported

 

 

 

 

available)

 

quisition of

tion of motor skills

descriptive

 

 

 

 

 

 

motor skills

in infants

data

 

 

 

 

 

 

including

 

- Failure to

 

 

 

 

 

 

rolling,

 

report the

 

 

 

 

 

 

crawling,

 

frequency of

 

 

 

 

 

 

moving on

 

using baby

 

 

 

 

 

 

hands and

 

walker

 

 

 

 

 

 

feet, sitting

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

without and

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

with help,

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

standing

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

and walking

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

dependently

 

 

Garret et al

Cohort study

190

Until the

26 weeks

Not reported

The onset

- Delay in achiev-

- Not re-

[23]

(retrospec-

 

onset of

(Median

 

age of head

ing normal

porting the

 

tive)

 

walking

age)

 

control

developmental

frequency of

 

 

 

 

 

 

during

milestones such as

using walker

 

 

 

 

 

 

prone,

crawling, standing

- Failure to

 

 

 

 

 

 

sitting with

and walking inde-

confirm the

 

 

 

 

 

 

support and

pendently, in baby

results of the

 

 

 

 

 

 

alone, crawl-

walker user.

study

 

 

 

 

 

 

ing, standing

 

- Failure to

 

 

 

 

 

 

with support

 

randomize

 

 

 

 

 

 

and alone,

 

the study

 

 

 

 

 

 

walking

 

participants

 

 

 

 

 

 

with support

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

and alone

 

 

Siegel and Burton [13]

Prospective cohort study

109

3 months until onset of walking

4.8 months (mean age at walker onset)

2.3. hours per day

Age at onset of sitting, crawling and walking (us- ing Bayley motor and developmen- tal scores)

-Delay motor de- velopment related to sitting, crawl- ing and walking in walker users

showed lower Bay- ley III scores

  • Various ages at onset of using walkers
  • Various frequencies of using walkers

*Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS); Ages & Stages Questionnaires-3 (ASQ-3); Ankara Developmental Screening Inventory (ADSI)

                        Table: Summary of Studies Characteristics and Outcomes

 

Study, Participant and Intervention Characteristics
Study Characteristics


In the current review, the study characteristics of the included stud- ies varied widely in terms of design. Seven out of eleven studies were observational cross-sectional studies, two studies were de- scribed as clinical trial, and other study was longitudinal study, and the last one semi-prospective case-control study. Using the baby walker during the first 18 months of child life is a common intervention taken by parent to aid in their motor development. However, Studies have shown that the use of baby walkers may have negative effect on the child’s functional motor development. Some of the expected outcomes of using a baby walker include developmental delay, child gait disorders, parents’ attitude and knowledge, parents’ satisfaction, hindered acquisition of early walking skills, enhancement of the mental, fine motor and sensory skills. Research has also shown a documented association between the use of baby walkers and infant motor development, with some studies reporting high levels of parental satisfaction.

Participants Characteristics
The total sample sizes were reported, with interventions were de- signed for healthy infants aged 0-18 months. Notably, one study did include participants between the ages of 18-30 months.

 Key Intervention Characteristics
Interventions aimed at using baby walkers often focus on measur- ing the average age at which children acquire certain motor skills, as well as providing the parents with targeted information. The frequency with which baby walkers are used with children can vary significantly, ranging from 30 minutes once/day to over 240 minutes daily. The interventions are typically followed up until the child begins walking independently.

Characteristics Most Frequently Associated with Signif- icant Improvements
The findings presented in table shed light on the impact of baby walkers on infant motor development. The data revealed that two studies showed a significant improvement in outcomes, the overall mean improvement in outcomes across studies was only 18%. Ad- ditionally, four articles did not show a significant improvement in outcome measures, highlighting the limited effectiveness of baby walkers. Six studies only reported that the use of baby walkers delayed the achievement of normal motor development in infants. The type of intervention also seemed to negatively influence infant motor development by 54.5%, with four studies indicating that it could result in gait disorders such as toe walking and flat foot, which were found to be more prevalent among baby walker us- ers compared to non-users. The literature review emphasized the frequency and duration of using baby walkers influencing chil- dren’s improvement. Ultimately, these results confirm that the use of baby walkers is 54% inefficient for children’s motor develop- ment, with a significant negative impact on their gait and overall development. Additionally, parents and caregivers should be more aware of these findings to make informed decisions regarding the use of baby walkers with infants.

Discussion

Claims have been made that use of baby walkers may result in delays in the development locomotor function [10]. Shafeek and El-Negmy discovered that 56.3% of children in Egypt use baby walker frequently, indicating a high rate of usage in Egyptian population [19]. Similarly, also Shiva et al, found that 54.5% of Iranian children use baby walkers [21]. Many studies conducted worldwide have reported a high prevalence of baby walker usage. In urban areas of Turkey, Dogan et al, observed a usage percentage of 75.4%, while in Ireland; Garrett et al, found that 54% of chil- dren use baby walkers [23,24]. Thein et al, reported a usage rate of 50% in the United Kingdom, and Marcella and McDonald docu- mented a usage rate of 70-90% in the United States [10]. Majority of parents who utilized walkers for their infants did so with the belief that it would help the infant in reaching the developmental milestone of independent walking at an earlier stage.

There has been a lot of research performed in various countries in different years on assessing influence exerted by baby walkers on motor skills and the results are rather controversial. This review included eleven articles examining the influence of the baby walk- er on the infants’ motor development. While some of the studies suggest that using baby walker may delay locomotion function and cognition in infant, there is limited research supporting these claims. The current evidence is insufficient to justify banning the use of baby walkers [12]. Regarding child development, Badihian et al., concluded a systematic review on the effect of the walker on motor development [4]. They found that there was insufficient and conflicting evidence on the effects of using a walker, suggesting they caution should be exercised until further studies with higher levels of evidence are concluded. Burrows and Griffiths also con- cluded a second review, pooling their analysis of various studies, and found a delay of 11 to 26 days in the acquisition of indepen- dent walking [12]. However, they remained uncertain about the significance of this delay.

Yaghini et al., the relationship of ASQ test result and baby walker usage among 107 infants was assessed [16]. The findings indicated there was no significant developmental delay among infants who used baby. This study marks the first attempts to examine the re- lationship of ASQ test result and baby walker usage in existing literature. However, previous studies have yielded contradictory outcomes. Garret et al., a study with large sample size 190 children identified that there was a relationship between the frequency of the baby use and extended of developmental delay [23]. The chil- dren used baby walker acquired motor milestones such as crawl- ing, standing alone and walking alone later than children who did not use baby walker. However, the reliability of these findings is called into question due to their significant issues. Nonetheless, Siegel and Burton support these findings [13]. Their prospective cohort study indicated that the use of occluding-walker, which are modern walkers that prevent infants from seeing their legs, delay the development of sitting and walking skills. Conversely, the see- feet walker, which are traditional walkers that allow babies to see their legs, did not have a significant impact on motor development. These studies had a more robust design compared to other obser- vational studies, with relatively large sample size and evaluation of child development using two distinct methods (ASQ and Bay- ley development Scale). Furthermore, these findings are consistent with the longitudinal study conducted by Talebian et al., which also found a significant delay in the acquisition of motor skills in a baby walker group [22]. Kauffman and Ridenour concluded that while the use of baby walkers may lead to temporary mild motor delay, these delays not relevant in real life situations for normal infants [25].

On the other hand, despite the delayed onset of crawling in the baby walker user group, there was no significant difference in the onset of independent walking between the users and non-users [26]. Three other studies also showed no difference on the age of gait acquisition between these two groups [11,18,21]. The findings of these studies were found to be contradictory to Shafeek and El-Negmy, which suggested a delay in acquisition of independent walking among walker [19]. This delay was found to be great- er with increased use of baby walkers, resulting in a delay of up to 16 days compared to lower walker users. This opposed to the latest studies Janusz et al., revealing an earlier onset of walking skills development among users, Alessa et al., have asserted that the use of baby walkers can actually improve infant’s motor de- velopment and potentially lead to the skipping of significant gross motor milestones [3,20].

Some of the researchers have suggest that the use of baby walkers may lead to tiptoe walking [3,18]. Moreover, there have been lim- ited number of studies examining the differences in gait patterns between baby walker users and non-users, with varying evaluation methods included subjective evaluations based on researcher ob- servations, which result in weak evidence to support their findings [25,27]. More studies that are rigorous contradict the idea that baby walkers lead to developmental delays, while larger observational studies have less methodological issues suggest that there may be a connection. Insufficient and conflicting evidence regarding the negative effects of baby walkers on child development makes it difficult to draw a definitive conclusion. It recommended using baby walkers cautiously until further robust studies are conducted on the matter. Multiple obstacles were faced during this study, ini- tially, stemming from inconsistencies in outcome and definitions across the articles reviewed, leading to the need to consolidate re- sults for analysis. Secondly, despite the prevalent correlation be- tween injuries and use of baby walkers, there was a lack of review on injuries related to walkers in the current paper assessed.

Conclusions

There is a significant deficiency in evidence regarding the potential influence of baby walkers’ child motor development. Some studies suggest that baby walkers may aid in motor development, while others indicated potential disadvantages such as delaying infants’ gait acquisition and causing gait disturbances in normal children. The existing literature does not provide sufficient data to outright ban the use of baby walkers; however, it does not demonstrate any clear advantages for child motor development either. This matter warrants greater attention from researchers to assist parents in making informed decisions for their children and to guide pedia- trician in advising their patient on this topic [28,29].

References

1.    Coats, T. J., & Allen, M. (1991). Baby walker related inju- ries--a continuing problem. Emergency Medicine Journal, 8(1), 52-55.
2.    Mulyani, N., & Budiarti, Y. (2022). Rancangan Alat Bantu Stimulasi Berjalan untuk Meningkatkan Kemampuan Ber- jalan Anak Usia 9–15 Bulan di Kota Tasikmalaya. Jurnal Riset Kesehatan Poltekkes Depkes Bandung, 14(2), 402-408.
3.    Janusz, P., Pikulska, D., Kapska, N., Kaniowska, M., Darcz, M., Bykowski, B., ... & Shadi, M. (2023). Association be- tween baby walker use and infant functional motor develop- ment. Pediatric physical therapy, 35(2), 237-241.
4.    Badihian, S., Adihian, N., & Yaghini, O. (2017). The effect of baby walker on child development: a systematic review. Iranian journal of child neurology, 11(4), 1-6.
5.    Mete, M., DevecioÄ?lu, E., Boran, P., Yetim, A., Pazar, A., & Gökçay, G. (2017). Baby walker use and its consequences in a group of turkish children. Journal of Child, 17(4), 158-162.
6.    Herdiman, L., Susmartini, S., & Andriani, S. Y. (2024). Com- parison of Baby Walkers Against Toddler Walking Ability Us- ing Biomechanics Through Kinect Sensor and Force Sensing Resistor Measurements. JST (Jurnal Sains dan Teknologi), 13(1), 41-56.
7.    Kendrick, D., & Marsh, P. (1998). Babywalkers: prevalence of use and relationship with other safety practices. Injury Pre- vention, 4(4), 295-298.
8.    American Academy of Pediatrics. (2001). Injuries associated with infant walkers. Pediatrics, 108(3), 790-792.
9.    Sims, A., Chounthirath, T., Yang, J., Hodges, N. L., & Smith,
G. A. (2018). Infant Walker–Related Injuries in the United States. Pediatrics, 142(4), e20174332.
10.    Thein, M. M., Lee, J., Tay, V., & Ling, S. L. (1997). Infant walker use, injuries, and motor development. Injury Preven- tion, 3(1), 63-66.
11.    Chagas, P. S., Mancini, M. C., Tirado, M. G., Megale, L., & Sampaio, R. F. (2011). Beliefs about the use of baby walkers. Brazilian Journal of Physical Therapy, 15, 303-309.
12.    Burrows, P., & Griffiths, P. (2002). Do baby walkers delay onset of walking in young children? British journal of com- munity nursing, 7(11), 581-586.
13.    Siegel, A. C., & Burton, R. V. (1999). Effects of baby walkers on motor and mental development in human infants. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 20(5), 355-360.
14.    Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., Altman, D. G., & PRIS- MA Group*, T. (2009). Preferred reporting items for system- atic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. An- nals of internal medicine, 151(4), 264-269.
15.    Bezgin, S., Akkaya, K. U., Çelik, H. İ., Çamurdan, A. D., & Elbasan, B. (2021). Evaluation of the effects of using a baby walker on trunk control and motor development. Turkish ar- chives of pediatrics, 56(2), 159-163.
16.    Yaghini, O., Goodarzi, M., Khoei, S., & Shirani, M. (2020). Effect of baby walker use on developmental status based on Ages and Stages Questionnaire Score (ASQ). Iranian Journal of Child Neurology, 14(1), 105-111.
17.    Mete, M., Keskindemirci, G., & Gokçay, G. (2019). Baby walker use and child development. Int. J. Pediatr. Res, 5(1),051-056.
18.    Sharov, A. N., Krivova, A. V., & Rodionova, S. S. (2018). Ap- plication of baby walkers in russia: Epidemiological aspects. Health Risk Analysis, (3), 63-75.
19.    Shafeek, M. M., & El-Negmy, E. H. (2016). Effect of usage of baby walker on acquisition and pattern of independent gait in normal children. Trends in Medical Research, 11(2), 76-81.
20.    Alessa, M., Humoud, M., & Al Qabandi, W. (2015). Parental attitudes toward the use of baby walkers. Int J Health Sci, 3, 109-113.
21.    Shiva, F., Ghotbi, F., & Yavari, S. F. (2010). The use of baby walkers in Iranian infants. Singapore medical journal, 51(8), 645.
22.    Talebian, A., HONARPISHEH, A., Taghavi, A., Fakharian, E., Parsa, M., & MOUSAVI, S. G. A. (2008). DO INFANTS USING BABYWALKERS SUFFER DEVELOPMENTAL DELAYS IN ACQUISITION OF MOTOR SKILLS?.
23.    Garrett, M., McElroy, A. M., & Staines, A. (2002). Locomo- tor milestones and babywalkers: cross sectional study. Bmj, 324(7352), 1494.
24.    Dogan, D. G., Bilici, M., Yilmaz, A. E., Catal, F., & Keles, N. (2009). Baby walkers: a perspective from Turkey. Acta paedi- atrica, 98(10), 1656-1660.
25.    Kauffman, I. B., & Ridenour, M. (1977). Influence of an infant walker on onset and quality of walking pattern of locomotion: an electromyographic investigation. Perceptual and motor skills, 45(3_suppl), 1323-1329.
26.    Crouchman, M. (1986). The effects of babywalkers on early locomotor development. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 28(6), 757-761.
27.    Engelbert, R. H., Van Empelen, R., Scheurer, N. D., Hel- ders, P. J., & Van Nieuwenhuizen, O. (1999). Influence of infant-walkers on motor development: mimicking spastic di- plegia? European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 3(6), 273- 275.
28.    Al-Nouri, L., & Al-Isami, S. A. (2006). Baby walker injuries. Annals of tropical paediatrics, 26(1), 67-71.
29.    Lasky, R. E. (1977). The effect of visual feedback of the hand on the reaching and retrieval behavior of young infants. Child development, 112-117.